Guitar Fingering For Music Performance Page 4

ADVERTISEMENT

Recall that both the costs of locality, fret stretch and
any skilled musician. As earlier noted in [5], familiar fin-
vertical stretch and the value of α were determined exper-
gerings may be adopted for new –similar– passages (e.g.,
imentally, on the base of several runs of the implemented
we refer to the practice of the transposition, which on fret-
model, as those leading to the average best results. The
ted instruments is a widespread habit). Chords and arpeg-
gap between the weights obtained is mainly due to the
gios may be part of musical patterns together with melodic
locality factor (that is, respectively 3, 27.75 and 13.5).
fragments, and such patterns may be learned, stored and
Hence, the first fingering will be chosen as the predeces-
retrieved as blocks. Cognitive strategies leading to choose
sor for all the fingered positions corresponding to the next
fingerings already known may have suggested the perfor-
note (Figure 3), which is the note D4, and this implies that
mer some fingerings that under the exclusive bio-mecha-
somehow the other two fingerings are pruned. If we don’t
nical aspect are not always preferable, and this could shed
want to loose the fingering solutions starting from (rooted
light on a significant part of the incorrect predictions of
in) the second or the third fingering, it is possible to reduce
the model.
the relative strength of the locality by means of a smaller
Future work will address such higher order issues to
α: i.e., by setting α = 0.05, we would yield the locality
further improve the model’s results, and the automatic anal-
factors 0.6, 5.55 and 2.7.
ysis field, which can give fruitful contributions to the fin-
gering problem as well.
4. EXPERIMENTAL VALIDATION
6. REFERENCES
We have considered the written fingerings of a guitarist,
[1] Cabral, G., Zanforlin, I., Lima, R., San-
bachelor in guitar performance on three pieces from the
tana, H., Ramalho, G. ”Playing along with
nineteenth Century didactic repertoire of the classical gui-
D’Accord Guitar”, Procs. of the 8
Brazil-
tar: the study n.3, from the guitar method of Aguado (1843),
ian Symposium on Computer Music, Fortaleza,
which is a simple study on chords , the study Opus 60 n.7
CE, 2001.
by Carcassi, which consists of a melody with an arpeg-
gio accompaniment, and the Siciliana Opus 121 n.15 by
[2] Dechter, R. Constraint processing, Morgan
Carulli, which combines chordal and melodic passages.
Kaufmann, San Francisco, CA, 2003.
We have requested the human expert to indicate the
[3] Heijink, H., Meulenbroek, R.G.J. ”On the
phrasing of each piece on the score, and then to mark
Complexity of Classical Guitar Playing: Func-
the fingering that would have adopted in a public perfor-
tional Adaptations to Task Constraints”, J. Mo-
mance. The same pieces were given in input to the im-
tor Behav., 34, 339–351, 2002.
plemented fingering model, with the phrasing annotated.
After the guitarist ended his task, he was requested to in-
[4] Jacobs, J.P. ”Refinements to the ergonomic
dicate whether in any passage the fingering computed by
model for keyboard fingering of Parncutt, Slo-
the model was not practicable.
boda, Clarke, Raekellio, and Desain”, Music
Results. The fingerings computed by the model are
Percept., 18, 505–511, 2001.
recognized to be viable, even though two automatically
[5] Parncutt, R., Sloboda, J.A., Clarke, E.,
fingered phrases were judged very hard to play. This de-
Raekallio, M., Desain, P. ”An ergonomic
monstrates that the set of constraints is adequate at some
model of keyboard fingering for melodic frag-
high level. The correct fingered positions are 90.61% (re-
ments”, Music Percept., 14, 341–382, 1997.
spectively 95.75%, 87.69%, 88.41%). The average num-
ber of missed fingers differences (that is, correct posi-
[6] Radicioni, D., Anselma, L., Lombardo, V. ”A
tion but wrong finger) has been 9.05% (4.25%, 12.31%,
prototype to compute string instruments fin-
10.59%), and the average number of wrong positions has
gering”, Procs. of the Conference on Inter-
been 2.18% (0%, 4.14% and 2.4%).
disciplinary Musicology, Graz, Austria, 2004,
cim04/.
5. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION
[7] Radicioni, D., Lombardo, V. ”Computational
modeling of chord fingering for string instru-
On the whole, the model obtained encouraging results,
ments”, Procs. of the 27
Annual Conference
even though a complete test would require several per-
of the Cognitive Science Society, Stresa, Italy,
formers and a refined metric of distances.
2005.
A closer analysis of the differences reveals that the mo-
[8] Sayegh, S. ”Fingering for String Instrument
del does not still account for some ’idiomatic’ gestures:
with the Optimum Path Paradigm”, Comput.
over the 50% (over the three pieces) of the differences
Music J., 13, 76–84, 1989.
were found on scales and chords played by arpeggio. In
some cases the fingerings provided by the model were
[9] Sloboda, J.A. The Musical Mind. The Cogni-
not strictly worse –under the bio-mechanical aspect–, than
tive Psychology of Music, Oxford, Clarendon
those provided by the expert, but the expert commented
Press, 1985.
that actually they would not have been even considered by

ADVERTISEMENT

00 votes

Related Articles

Related forms

Related Categories

Parent category: Life
Go
Page of 4