Linguistic Development Research Paper Page 21

ADVERTISEMENT

8 THE TELEGRAPHIC PERIOD
21
Semantic Relation
Examples
Agent + Action
mommy come; daddy sit
Action + Object
drive car; eat grape
Agent + Object
mommy sock; baby book
Action+ Location
go park; sit chair
Entity + Location
cup table; toy floor
Possessor + Possessed
my Teddy; mommy dress
Entity + Attribute
box shiny; crayon big
Demonstrative + Entity
dat money; dis telephone
Table 5: Common categories of meaning (semantic relations) expressed in children’s
earliest sentences
they are learning. The semantic rules which it is born with can be combined in a variety of ways
which can reflect the language community in which it is born.
In the prelinguistic stage prior to the development of intelligent speech babies have still developed
a means of communicating with their care-givers. e.g. crying, cooing, laughing, whining and partic-
ularly pointing. Perhaps by pointing at an object the mother is prompted to give a name to that
object. So the use of pointing and other gestures may serve the purpose of increasing the linguistic
input that the child receives.
The babies will also be using other gestures to disambiguate telegraphic sentences. By 2 to 2 ; 6
children have also learnt that there are other keys to effective communication, such as hearing others
properly by placing yourself near them and making yourself heard by speaking up.
The order order of development is fairly predictable and is the same for all languages that share
the same system of noun and verb suffixes. Until children have learnt suffixes that make word order
flexible they have no way to produce unambiguous word orderings.
Towards age 3 the child is able to determine the subject and object of a sentence without having
to make reference to the word ordering. They are able to do this by examining the relationships
between the verb and the other sentence parts. As is described in other parts of this report the
cognitively simplest grammatical morphemes are learnt first. For instance the present tense third
person singular /-s/ as in “he goes”m develops before the past tense endings such as “he goed”.
There are also less variations in the structure of the present tense third person singular forms of
verbs than there are in the past tense forms.
From 1 ; 6 onwards the child is already beginning to produce two word sentences, even though
single word sentences continue to be used for some time. At 1 ; 6 the baby is becoming skilled at
monitoring and comprehending the responses of others, even though they have not really mastered
the grammatical rules of language.
At 1 ; 9 to 2 ; 0 the child is able to adapt their form of communication if they are not able to make
themselves understood (repair). By introducing flexibility to find some means of communicating the
child is actively seeking out a rudimentary syntactic/semantic system.
By 2 ; 0 they are regularly producing 3 to 4 word sentences, often ungrammatical by adult standards
but certainly obeying some rules of grammar. The rules are fewer and simpler but that have been
shown to exist. Their understanding of the grammatical rules of language used by adults is expanding
very quickly. They will start to reorder the words in sentences in accordance with syntactic rules for
the conversion of declarative sentences to questions or negations etc. The children will at this stage
start to practice dialogue when they are alone. It seems that they are practicing new words and
grammar forms and playing with sounds and rhymes. They are trying to make sense of the world

ADVERTISEMENT

00 votes

Related Articles

Related forms

Related Categories

Parent category: Education